[R-lang] Re: Main effects of categorical predictors in lmer

T. Florian Jaeger tiflo@csli.stanford.edu
Tue Oct 11 14:06:41 PDT 2011


On Tue, Oct 11, 2011 at 7:51 AM, hossein karimi <karimihussein@gmail.com>wrote:

> Hi Florian,
>
> Thank you very much for your help. I did what you suggested and got
> significant main effects for A and B as well as their interaction. Just one
> question out of curiosity: To obtain main effects, can I also compare a
> 'base' model (including only random slopes) and then add the predictors one
> by one to see whether they improve the model significantly? I mean,
> something like this:
> l.base = lmer(response ~ (1 + A | sub) + (1 | item), data,
> family="binomial")
> l.A = lmer(response ~ A + (1 + A | sub) + (1 | item), data,
> family="binomial")
> l.AB = lmer(response ~ A+B + (1 + A | sub) + (1 | item), data,
> family="binomial")
> l.full = lmer(response ~ A+B+A:B + (1 | sub) + (1 | item), data,
> family="binomial")
>
> And then, I can go 'anova(I.base, I.A, I.AB, I.full)'. I did this and got
> the same results. I was just wondering if there is a difference between this
> and what you suggested
>

the order in which the models are fitted does not change the results.

B matters if l.AB is better than l.A or if l.full is better than l.AB (since
then you have at least an interaction, perhaps more).

If you have a main effect or *just *an interaction is harder to test this
way: you can test whether you have an interaction by comparing l.AB vs.
l.full, but significance of l.AB compared to l.A or significance of l.B
compared to l.base does *not *guarantee a 'main effect' in the sense of an
ANOVA (since B might matter due to the interaction). However, if A and B
were perfectly balanced and therefore orthogonal in their effects, then
significance of l.B vs. l.base already shows you that there is a main effect
of B (same for l.AB vs. l.A).

hth,

Florian




On Mon, Oct 10, 2011 at 9:07 PM, T. Florian Jaeger
<tiflo@csli.stanford.edu>wrote:

> Hi Hossein,
>
> so, let's take this one step at a time. Here's what you can do to answer
> whether A or B or both have an effect (I am going to assume that the random
> effect structure you used, (1 + A | sub) + (1 | item), has been shown to be
> the appropriate one or at least sufficient; if not adapt accordingly):
>
> l.full = lmer(response ~ A + B + A:B + (1 + A | sub) + (1 | item), data,
> family="binomial")
> l.AB = lmer(response ~ A + B + (1 + A | sub) + (1 | item), data,
> family="binomial")
> l.A = lmer(response ~ A + (1 + A | sub) + (1 | item), data,
> family="binomial")
> l.B = lmer(response ~ B + (1 | sub) + (1 | item), data, family="binomial")
>
> if anova(l.full, l.A) is significant, B has an effect (main effect or
> interaction).
> if anova(l.full, l.B) is significant, A has an effect (main effect or
> interaction).
>
>  (If there are not significant, non-significance of A or B, respectively,
> does *not* follow). Given what you've shown us already, both will be
> significant. Then
>
> if anova(l.full, l.AB) is significant, you have a significant interaction
> of A and B.
>
> This will also be the case given what you've shown us so far. You don't
> technically need to do this. But I wanted to let you know what you would
> need to do to get the closest equivalent to the so-called omnibus test that
> the ANOVA would provide.
>
> Now, the regression model (if appropriately coded) also gives
> you interpretable information about the contrasts between the three levels
> of B. Which coding I would a priori use is hard to tell since you chose to
> abstract away from your real data (A and B, rather than NP and .... ;)).
> Btw, it usually helps if you describe your actual data and provide e;g.
> head(data[,relevantcolumns]).
>
> With regard to your email:
>
> > contrasts(B)
>>                  [,1] [,2]
>> B2               0    1
>> B1              -1   -1
>> B3               1    0
>>
>> The three levels of B are coded with names rather than numbers and the
>> baseline condition of B alphabetically comes second; that's why I manually
>> recoded the matrix so that the baseline receives -1. However, as you can
>> see, this matrix is different from the one Florian posted. I used the
>> following code to create the contrasts for B (but I just read Ian's message
>> which says this is wrong):
>>
>> > contrasts (B)<- contr.sum(3)
>> > contrasts(B) <- cbind(c(0,-1,1),c(1,-1,0))
>>
>
> so here the second row overrides the first! Here's what I recommend:
>
> # order of levels argument matters!
>  B = factor(B, levels = ("B1", "B2", "B3"))
> contrasts(B) = contr.helmert(3) / 2
> # dividing by 2 to give you distances of 1 between group means
>
> which will give you:
>
> > contrasts(B)
> B1   -.5   -.5
> B2    .5   -.5
> B3    0    1
>
> which is what you seem to want. Contrast 'B1' will contrast B==2 against
> the baseline B==1. Contrast 'B2' will contrast B==3 against the mean of B==1
> and B==2.
>
> I recommend you contrast code A:
>
> A = factor(A, levels = ("baseline", "notbaseline"))
> contrasts(B) = contr.sum(2) / 2
>
>  > contrasts(A)
> A1   -.5
> A2    .5
>
> Then l.full (above) should be rather interpretable.
>
> Florian
>
>>
>> So, if I want to compare the baseline condition of B with the mean of
>> other two conditions of B, I need to create contrasts like Florian's? When I
>> helmet code the levels of B with the baseline receiving 2 and the other two
>> levels of B receiving -1, I get the following confusing contrasts. I have no
>> idea what this means. I’m just hoping that I’m comparing the baseline with
>> the mean of the other B levels:
>> > contrasts (helmertB)
>>      2
>> -1  0
>> 2   1
>>
>>
>> And here is the complete output of the first model (where A is numerical
>> and centered):
>>
>>
>> Generalized linear mixed model fit by the Laplace approximation
>> Formula: response ~ A * B + (A + 1 | sub) + (1 | item)
>>    Data: mydata
>>    AIC   BIC logLik deviance
>>  745.1 794.6 -362.6    725.1
>> Random effects:
>>
>>  Groups Name        Variance Std.Dev. Corr
>>  item   (Intercept) 0.93903  0.96903
>>  sub    (Intercept) 2.39079  1.54622
>>         A                0.34163  0.58449  0.551
>> Number of obs: 1038, groups: item, 42; sub, 36
>>
>> Fixed effects:
>>                  Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)
>> (Intercept)        1.1661     0.3285   3.550 0.000385 ***
>> A                     -2.0741     0.1690 -12.270  < 2e-16 ***
>> B1                   -0.4166     0.1580  -2.636 0.008389 **
>> B2                    -0.2867     0.1783  -1.608 0.107842
>> A:B1                 0.9881     0.1594   6.199 5.68e-10 ***
>> A:B2                 -0.7101     0.1750  -4.059 4.94e-05 ***
>> ---
>>
>> Signif. codes:  0 ‘***’ 0.001 ‘**’ 0.01 ‘*’ 0.05 ‘.’ 0.1 ‘ ’ 1
>>
>> Correlation of Fixed Effects:
>>                   (Intr)     NP     lngt.1 lngt.2 NP:l.1
>> A                0.121
>> B1              -0.173  0.251
>> B2              0.003  0.060 -0.281
>> A:B1           0.141 -0.304 -0.259 -0.095
>> A:B2           0.021  0.056 -0.081 -0.300 -0.334
>>
>>  Florian, you already interpreted this output but it was based on the
>> your assumption of the contrasts matrix for B…
>>
>>
>> By the way, I didn't quite get Ian's suggestion for A. Should I keep it
>> numerical and centered?
>>
>> Many thanks,
>>
>> hossein
>>
>>
>> On Mon, Oct 10, 2011 at 6:11 PM, Finlayson, Ian <IFinlayson@qmu.ac.uk>wrote:
>>
>>> **
>>>
>>> You probably are thinking in the ANOVA way (will leave that for others to
>>> decide if that's good/bad, appropriate/innappropriate) but anova(model) will
>>> give you an ANOVA-style table for your model (minus the p values).
>>>
>>> Not entirely sure what you mean about having added NP, I assume you added
>>> it as another fixed effect, but it's not listed in your results, so you may
>>> have meant something else. If B2 becomes significant after including NP then
>>> that just suggests to me that NP cleans up noise in your data, allowing B2
>>> to become significant (can't help but notice that the p value in the first
>>> model is only just over .1, so it's not massively far from significance).
>>>
>>> I should have made the point earlier, but I will now: If you're actually
>>> interested in whether there's a difference between B1 and B2, and between B1
>>> and B3, then sum coding isn't appropriate. Sum coding will tell you if
>>> there's a difference between the levels of the factor and the grand mean. If
>>> you actually want to compare B1 with B2, and B3, then you'll need to use
>>> treatment coding. Your model suggests this is experimental data (Subject,
>>> Item etc.) so I'll assume it's a roughly balanced design and collinearity
>>> might not be a huge concern.
>>>
>>> You're correct that you could use subsetting (dividing the data according
>>> to A makes sense to me, but that is obviously atheoretical as I have no idea
>>> what A represents) to get a clear idea of your pattern of results. Although,
>>> as I've just noticed Florian showing, you can get a good picture if you
>>> understand the coding and work through the coefficients.
>>>
>>> HTH,
>>>
>>> Ian
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>> -----Original Message-----
>>> From: hossein karimi [mailto:karimihussein@gmail.com<karimihussein@gmail.com>
>>> ]
>>> Sent: Mon 10/10/2011 16:15
>>> To: Finlayson, Ian
>>> Cc: ling-r-lang-l@mailman.ucsd.edu
>>> Subject: Re: [R-lang] Main effects of categorical predictors in lmer
>>>
>>> Hi Ian,
>>>
>>> Thank you for your help.I did what you suggested and got the following
>>> output:
>>> Fixed effects:
>>>                  Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)
>>> (Intercept)        1.1661     0.3285   3.550 0.000385 ***
>>> A                    -2.0741     0.1690 -12.270  < 2e-16 ***
>>> B1                   -0.4166     0.1580  -2.636 0.008389 **
>>> B2                   -0.2867     0.1783  -1.608 0.107842
>>> A:B1                 0.9881     0.1594   6.199 5.68e-10 ***
>>> A:B2                -0.7101     0.1750  -4.059 4.94e-05 ***
>>>
>>> I think this is telling me there is a main effect of A, but my problem is
>>> that I also need to report a main effect of B, and that's something I
>>> don't
>>> find in this output. I might be thinking in the old ANOVA way, but I
>>> think I
>>> should report main effects as well as a main interaction (if there is
>>> any)
>>> and then if there's a significant interaction, I should proceed with
>>> breaking down the main interaction to see where exactly it lies, am I
>>> right?
>>> or, are lmer results reported differently?
>>>
>>> By the way, I also ran a model with NP as a factor too and got the
>>> following
>>> result:
>>> Fixed effects:
>>>                   Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)
>>> (Intercept)         0.9371     0.3313   2.829  0.00468 **
>>> A1                   2.0857     0.1700  12.269  < 2e-16 ***
>>> B1                   -0.3075     0.1544  -1.991  0.04646 *
>>> B2                    -0.3651     0.1735  -2.105  0.03533 *
>>> A1:B1              -0.9936     0.1603  -6.199 5.68e-10 ***
>>> A1:B2               0.7139     0.1759   4.058 4.96e-05 ***
>>>
>>> The effect of B2 is significant in the first model above but not in
>>> second!!! why is that?
>>>
>>> Many thanks,
>>>
>>> Hossein
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>>
>>> On Mon, Oct 10, 2011 at 3:27 PM, Finlayson, Ian <IFinlayson@qmu.ac.uk
>>> >wrote:
>>>
>>> > Hi Hossein,****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>>
>>> >
>>> > As far as I can see, neither model is correct. If B is a predictor with
>>> > three levels then the model should contain 6 fixed effects: An
>>> intercept, A,
>>> > B1 compared to B2, B1 compared to B3, B1 compared to B2 interacting
>>> with A,
>>> > and B1 compared to B3 interacting with A.****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>>
>>> >
>>> > What I suspect you've done is actually enter B as a continuous
>>> predictor
>>> > with a mean of zero (since you used scale()). You'll need to keep B as
>>> a
>>> > factor, but give it appropriate contrasts:****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > B <- factor(B) # Make sure it's a factor****
>>>
>>> >
>>> > contrasts(B) <- contr.sum(3) # Generate a 3 level contrast matrix using
>>> sum
>>> > coding; alternative contr.helmert(3) for Helmhert coding****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > Then build your models.****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>>
>>> >
>>> > It's possible that it won't set the right level as a baseline (the
>>> baseline
>>> > will be whichever is a row of -1). If so then you'll need to manually
>>> set up
>>> > the matrix. For sum coding, something like:****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>>
>>> >
>>> > contrasts(B) <- cbind(c(-1,0,1),c(-1,1,0)) # Assuming that B1 is the
>>> first
>>> > row in contrasts(B) - double check!****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>>
>>> >
>>> > I'd also do something similar with A so that your estimates are
>>> comparable
>>> > (if you've centered 0 and 1, then it'll come out -.5 and .5, rather
>>> than -1
>>> > and 1)****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > HTH,****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > Ian****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > *From:* ling-r-lang-l-bounces@mailman.ucsd.edu [mailto:
>>> > ling-r-lang-l-bounces@mailman.ucsd.edu] *On Behalf Of *hossein karimi
>>> > *Sent:* 10 October 2011 15:05
>>> > *To:* ling-r-lang-l@mailman.ucsd.edu
>>> > *Subject:* [R-lang] Main effects of categorical predictors in lmer****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > Dear R users,****
>>>
>>> >
>>> > I'm using mixed effects models (lmer) to predict a binary dependent
>>> > variable as a function of 1.a categorical predictor (A)with 2 levels
>>> (A1 and
>>> > A2) , 2. another categorical predictor (B) with three levels (B1, B2
>>> and B3)
>>> > and 3. The interaction between these two predictors. I have tried two
>>> models
>>> > but they return different results and I'm not sure which one is
>>> correct. I'm
>>> > interested in the main effect of B and the interaction between A and B
>>> > (because A alone has a significant effect in both models). My problem
>>> is
>>> > that there seem to be two sensible ways of examining the main effect of
>>> B:
>>> > 1. to helmert code and 2. to center.  But these two methods produce
>>> opposite
>>> > results! I don't know which one I should use. Here are the two models
>>> with
>>> > some details and their outputs:****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>>
>>> >
>>> > Model 1: 'A' is centered. 'B' is helmert coded ('B1'(baseline)=2,
>>> 'B2'=-1,
>>> > 'B3'=-1) so that I can get a main effect of B by checking to see
>>> whether
>>> > baseline condition in B differs from the mean of B1 and B2 . The lmer
>>> output
>>> > returns a significant effect of B and no significant AxB interaction.
>>> > However, as is highlighted below (in pink), the correlation between B
>>> and
>>> > the 'AxB' interaction is high (-54%).  ****
>>> >
>>> >  ****
>>>
>>> >
>>> > > model.1<-lmer(response~A*B+(A+1|sub)+(1|item), mydata,
>>> family="binomial")
>>> > ****
>>> >
>>> > > print(model.1)****
>>> >
>>> > Generalized linear mixed model fit by the Laplace approximation ****
>>> >
>>> > Formula: response ~ A * B+ (A + 1 | sub) + (1 | item) ****
>>> >
>>> >    Data: mydata****
>>> >
>>> >  AIC   BIC logLik deviance****
>>> >
>>> > 783 822.6 -383.5      767****
>>> >
>>> > Random effects:****
>>> >
>>> > Groups Name        Variance Std.Dev. Corr  ****
>>> >
>>> >  item   (Intercept) 0.7293   0.85399        ****
>>> >
>>> >  sub    (Intercept) 2.0871   1.44468        ****
>>> >
>>> >         A          1.3812   1.17524  0.562 ****
>>> >
>>> > Number of obs: 1038, groups: item, 42; sub, 36****
>>> >
>>> >  ****
>>> >
>>> > Fixed effects:****
>>> >
>>> >                   Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)    ****
>>> >
>>> > (Intercept)        1.05261    0.30283   3.476 0.000509 *******
>>> >
>>> > A                -3.91080    0.32239 -12.131  < 2e-16 *******
>>> >
>>> > B                  0.36128    0.09751   3.705 0.000211 *******
>>> >
>>> > A:B            -0.29638    0.18681  -1.586 0.112626    ****
>>> >
>>> > ---****
>>> >
>>> > Signif. codes:  0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1 ****
>>> >
>>> >  ****
>>> >
>>> > Correlation of Fixed Effects:****
>>> >
>>> >                         (Intr)   A      B****
>>> >
>>> > A                  0.155              ****
>>> >
>>> > B                  0.160 -0.278       ****
>>> >
>>> > A:B              -0.156  0.238 -0.540****
>>> >
>>> >  ****
>>>
>>> >
>>> > Model 2: 'A' and 'B' are both centered. The lmer output returns no
>>> > significant effect of B but the A:B interaction is significant. The
>>> > correlations between predictors are generally lower and the correlation
>>> > between B and A:B is reduced to -26%. ****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > Generalized linear mixed model fit by the Laplace approximation ****
>>> >
>>> > Formula: resonse ~ A * B + (A + 1 | sub) + (1 | item) ****
>>> >
>>> >    Data: mydata ****
>>> >
>>> >    AIC   BIC logLik deviance****
>>> >
>>> > 756.1 795.7 -370.1    740.1****
>>> >
>>> > Random effects:****
>>> >
>>> > Groups Name        Variance Std.Dev. Corr  ****
>>> >
>>> >  item   (Intercept) 0.87028  0.93289        ****
>>> >
>>> >  sub    (Intercept) 2.41707  1.55469        ****
>>> >
>>> >         A         1.23669  1.11206  0.533 ****
>>> >
>>> > Number of obs: 1038, groups: item, 42; sub, 36****
>>> >
>>> >  ****
>>> >
>>> > Fixed effects:****
>>> >
>>> >                 Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)    ****
>>> >
>>> > (Intercept)       1.1004     0.3239   3.398 0.000679 *******
>>> >
>>> > A               -4.0941     0.3248 -12.605  < 2e-16 *******
>>> >
>>> > B                -0.1461     0.1400  -1.043 0.296851    ****
>>> >
>>> > A:B             1.7923     0.2818   6.360 2.01e-10 *******
>>> >
>>> > ---****
>>> >
>>> > Signif. codes:  0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1 ****
>>> >
>>> >  ****
>>> >
>>> > Correlation of Fixed Effects:****
>>> >
>>> >                        (Intr)      A        B****
>>> >
>>> > A                    0.138              ****
>>> >
>>> > B                   -0.148  0.185       ****
>>> >
>>> > A:B                0.106 -0.292 -0.265****
>>> >
>>> >  ****
>>>
>>> >
>>> > I personally think Model 2 is better but the thing is that I have
>>> centered
>>> > a categorical predictor with *three* levels. In my searches in the web,
>>> I
>>>
>>> > have never seen a three-level predictor to be centered; they were all
>>> > two-level categorical predictors. ****
>>>
>>> >
>>> > I have used the scale() function to center the predictors (I first
>>> > converted them to numeric variables and then used the scale () function
>>> to
>>> > center them). As I mentioned, my problem is that I don't know how to
>>> get a
>>> > main effect of B as well as a *main* A:B interaction. On the one hand,
>>> it
>>>
>>> > seems logical to compare 'B1' (baseline) with the mean of the other two
>>> B
>>> > conditions to see if the B manipulation has a general effect. On the
>>> other
>>> > hand, I hear that one needs to center variables to get a main effect.
>>> ****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > I would be grateful of you could please help me****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > Regards,****
>>> >
>>> > ** **
>>> >
>>> > Hossein****
>>> >
>>>
>>>
>>
>
-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: http://mailman.ucsd.edu/pipermail/ling-r-lang-l/attachments/20111011/bfc110fc/attachment-0001.html 


More information about the ling-r-lang-L mailing list